Introduction
With regard to the spatial behaviour of
the Common Swift, we have some knowledge of their nesting and feeding habits.
Cramp (1994) states (after Lack 1956) that the „nest-territory [is] confined
to immediate surroundings of nest; serves for part of courtship, some copulation,
and the raising of young”. Most observations relate to their feeding grounds
which are either near (Parmenter & Owen 1954, Brinkmann 1931 in Weitnauer
& Scherner (1994) or separate from their nesting sites (Cramp 1994,
Gory & Jeantet 1986, Gladwin & Nau 1964, Koskimies 1961, Cutcliffe
1955, Haartman 1949). Others have noticed the gatherings over inshore waters
under bad weather conditions (e.g. Lack & Owen 1955, Ries, in
Stadler 1917, Birk 1916, Dornig 1916) or in hunting fields on migration
(Trouche 1948). But the reports remain unclear as to the location of the
territory of the colony, in particular the organisation of periodical relationship
of Swifts during their stay in our part of the world. Cramp (1994) merely
mentions a „colony area”, in which the flock races around and calls. Kaiser
(1992) notes that some non-breeders „return each spring to their individual
territories“, Busche (1995) states that swarms of different origin circled
around specific places, and Gory 1994 mentions that his analysis of nesting
materials show them to have been „collected within a one kilometre radius
of the colony“. Offringa (1994) uses the term colony in a vague sense („vanzelfsprekend“)
while Tombal & Tombal (1995) consider it as a spacial grouping of Swifts,
seperated from each other, around a building, a street, a city or a quarter
and Colombo & Galeotti (1993) mention that „air-ranges of different
colonies showed no overlap and stranger individuals were driven off“. There
is, however, a lack of detailed information on the spatial behaviour of
the Common Swift.
In 1993 I first recognized of the concept
of a territory for Swifts (Tigges 1994). Instead of term “territory” I
then used the neutral word Kiez (quarter). At that time I was not quite
clear about the sense and marking of a territory which by international
consensus requires defence behaviour (e.g. Lawrence 1995, Hentschel &
Wagner 1990, Berndt & Winkel 1983, Dathe 1978). But in fact the colony
area of Swifts is a territory in its full meaning. It is a clearly defined
space which is regularly flown over by the members of the colony and which
is differentiated to foreigners of the same species through calls.
Locality and conditions of observation
The investigated colony of Swifts is located
in the centre of Berlin at co-ordinates 52°29’N 13°26’E. Although
this is a built-up area the surface is not completely covered by concrete
and roads: there are two small green spaces as well as numerous backyards
covered with bushes and trees; there are two playgrounds with surfaces
of sand and stone chippings; in all the streets there are rows of trees,
mostly adult limes; there is also a big car park, a school yard and a large
number of garages. The houses are mostly old buildings in good condition,
which are divided into four flats. About half a mile to the west is a municipal
park of about 50 hectares, which adjoins Tempelhof airport. Within the
park there is a newly built pond of about 3,200 square metres of
water surface, but this pond is rarely used by the Swifts for feeding or
drinking.
The territory of the Swifts
The
following observations arose from the settlement of one pair of Swifts
in a nesting box placed in a window of my 4th floor flat (see position
A on the map). 13 other Swifts gathered around this breeding pair, making
a colony of 15 Swifts. I was able to observe the flying activities
of this colony very intensively for 5 years. From 1991 until 1993 this
pair was the only breeding pair. Since 1994 there has probably been another
pair (position B on the map). What was remarkable was the high proportion
of non-breeders. In a neighbouring colony again just one pair bred in 1993
(Tigges 1994). This small group was easy to observe and identify, a lucky
requirement which would hardly be possible with higher numbers of individuals.
This colony of Swifts possessed a fixed territory of about 120.000 square
metres (ca. 30 acres). It consisted of a centre which was flown over by
the members nearly exclusively and a fringe which was used by members of
the adjacent colonies, too. This fringe extended over open space such as
a parking place and a school yard. The birds orientated themselves according
to the local topography and set the borderline as a line of houses, so
that in this case the territory had the form of a triangle with the sides
of length about 420 x 430 x 520 m .
Territorial behaviour
The rules of spatial and temporal behaviour
in the territory were as follows:
Timetable
Under good weather conditions the territory
was left around noon. The birds flew away between 11 am and 2 pm and got
back in the evening between 5 and 8 pm (CEST). The birds might go off for
feeding flights. Haartman (1949), Hess (1926) and Swaen (1922) report
the same behaviour. The observation that the birds reappeared a good half
an hour before sunset can be a hint of regional differences in behaviour
(Busche 1995).
Breeding period
After their arrival in the first two weeks
of May, the breeding pair occupied the old nest site. In 1993 the pair
in my home was the only breeding pair in the colony. Other birds, who arrived
at around the same time called on potential nesting sites and occupied
them or over-summered as non-breeders within or outside the territory.
According to my observations what was presumed to be yearlings (birds hatched
the previous summer) appeared in the second half of June. Their arrival
was apparent in that the stock of birds rose from about 7-8 to 14-15 individuals
and that the hunt for nesting places began once again.
The arrival of the yearlings has not yet
been systematically studied. My own observations correspond with those
of Kaiser (1992). On the strength of ring controls he states that the yearlings
arrived in his colony in Hesse in the second half of the month of June.
However, Weitnauer (1990) states that their arrival in his Swiss colony
coincided with that of the breeding adults. The yearlings adapt to the
new colony of their own choice relatively fast (Kaiser 1992). Until now
it has been unknown whether or not they move forward like the breeding
adults in geographically orientated waves (c.f. Sliwinski 1938).
The search for nesting sites by newcommers
principally takes place in their own territory. Tests with acoustic signals
showed that only that part of population which belongs to the colony respond
to the vocalizations of the prospecting birds. All the other Swifts showed
no reaction and moved towards the centre of their own colonies. In this
experiment duet calls recorded at a nesting site were taped to impersonate
an occupied nesting place. However, not all Swifts who could reacted to
these calls from their own species. The place of this experiment was only
some metres away from the borderline of the centre (cf. C on map) so that
I was able to watch those individuals of the neighbouring colony too, who
used the outer part of the territory, too. These birds did not show any
reaction to the acoustic signals and were exclusively oriented to their
own territory (Tigges 1995).
As of the second half of July, towards
the end of the Swifts’ stay with us, the frontiers lost their significance.
The young were either fledged or were just about to fledge. It was now
possible to observe more frequently than before that some members of the
colony left their territory for a short time to fly together with their
neighbours in the neighbouring territory, or conversely members of neighbouring
societies entered their territory for flying together. In spite of the
increased frequency of this mixing, the boundaries were still in force
which was evident both in the short time of gathering (during periods from
only a few seconds to a few minutes) and the respect for the area for the
rest of the time. The size of these amalgamated flying groups ranged from
between about 15 to 50 individual birds.
During this period there was a clear rise
in the frequency of visits to the nesting sites and the number of individual
birds appeared to be greater than during the rest of the stay. In this
case, it was not part of the regular search for suitable nesting places
but rather appeared to be a highly ritualised conduct for social purposes.
This behaviour seems to strengthen the sense of the common bond on the
one hand and show the locality to the potential future colony members.
Amongst these potential future colony members were probably both members
of neighbouring colonies as well as chicks (the current year’s fledglings).
Kaiser (1992) reported from his colony at the southern ridge of the Taunus
mountain that 40 % of the adult non-breeders return year after year to
the colony. Applying this same percentage to the local conditions leads
one to the expectation of about eight or nine newcomers every year, if
the size of the colony is calculated with 15 birds (from them two breeders
and six adult non-breeders in 1993, four breeders and four adult non-breeders
in 1994 and 1995, and each time seven yearlings).
Advantages of territory
The advantage of maintaining a territory
is to reduce intraspecific aggression. The discovery of a suitable nesting
site undoubtedly is the most important event in the life of a Swift. After
its return from Africa, the adult bird occupies at once or after a few
days its traditional nesting site (Lack 1958). Mature individuals who do
not yet have a nesting site begin with seeking nesting sites and try out
especially those places which they know from previous years, which generally
means those sites already occupied. This gives rise to the owners claiming
their right of the hole or nesting site, which they defend vigorously.
Aggression in Swifts is high. If fighting breaks out, it is often vicious
and, due to the method of fighting, including the use of claws, this often
leads to severe injuries or even to the death of one of the adversaries
(e.g. Kaiser 1992, Reuter et al. 1980, Roper 1960, Lack, 1956, Damm 1934,
Brehms Tierleben 1927, Lakowitz 1925). The explanation for the toughness
and stubbornness of fighting Swifts (c.f. Lack & Lack 1952) lies in
the breeding biology. Suitable holes, which are necessary for reproduction,
are only found in limited numbers in nature.
A similar situation arises once again after
the arrival of the yearlings. The fidelity to the breeding place is not
yet effective and their dispersion dynamic is relatively high. On their
common flights with the colony members they learn the territory borders,
thereby establishing their traditional area. It is only then within this
area that the search for a suitable nesting site occurs, thereby avoiding
any intraspecific fighting with birds from other areas, and limiting the
associated disturbance and stress.
In the five years of observations I could
not quote any change in the behaviour of the colony, so that one can assume
that under same exterior conditions a colony of Swifts is a stable unit
as far as the numbers of individuals as well as the territory borders are
concerned.
Keeping estate
On the basis of this new discovery one
can answer the question raised by the “screaming displays” (Cramp 1994)
or “flying screaming parties” (Hayman 1990, Lack & Lack 1952). They
declare the claim of ownership of the colony territory. There may well,
of course, be further meanings in these actions.
Acknowledgements
I am grateful to Henry Hahnke, Museum
of Natural History, Berlin for the discussions and Marc Wentworth, London
for the revision of the English text.
References
Berndt, Rudolf & Winkel, Wolfgang
(1983): Eco-ornithological Glossary. Berlin, p. 62
Birk, Joh. (1916): Abzug der Mauersegler.
Die Gefiederte Welt 45:271-272
Brehms Tierleben (1927), revised after
the second edition by Otto Evers. Vögel Vol. 1, Hamburg
Brinkmann (1931) in Weitnauer, Emil &
Scherner, Erwin R. 1994: Apus apus - Mauersegler.
In Glutz von Blotzheim & Bauer: Handbuch
der Vögel Mitteleuropas Vol. 9, p. 687, Wiesbaden
Busche, Günther (1995): Mauersegler
(Apus apus) in ländlichen Wohnbereichen im Westen Schleswig-Holsteins
- mit Vorschlägen zur Bestandsermittlung. Charadrius 31:19-22
Colombo, A. & Galeotti, P. (1993):
Nest-hole selection as defence measure in breeding Swifts (Apus apus).
Avocetta 17:1-6
Cramp, Stanley (Ed.)(1994): Handbook of
the Birds of Europe the Middle East and North Africa Vol. 4. Oxford-New
York, p. 657-670
Cutcliffe, A.S. (1955): Further notes
of the Swift, 1944-1954. British Birds 48:193-203
Damm, Willi (1934): Kämpfende Mauersegler.
Beiträge zur Fortpflanzungsbiologie der Vögel 10:191
Dathe, Holger H. in Tembrock, Günter
(Ed.) (1978): Verhaltensbiolo-gie. Jena, p. 201-202
Dornig, Heinrich (1916): Der wahrscheinliche
Ursprung der Benennung Regenschwalbe.
Aquila 23:513-515
Gladwin, T.W. & Nau, B.S. (1964):
A study of Swifts weights. British Birds 57:344-356
Gory, Gérard & Jeantet, René
(1986): La colonie de Martinet Noir (Apus apus L.) du Museum de Nîmes
compte rendu de 19 années de baguage. Bulletin de la Société
d’Études des Sciences Naturelles de Nîmes et du Gard
57:46-52
Gory, Gérard (1994): Recherche
et utilisation des matériaux nécessaires à la
construction du nid du Martinet noir Apus
apus. Alauda 62:117-122
Haartman, Lars von (1949): Neue Studien
über den Tagesrhythmus des Mauerseglers, Apus apus (L.). Ornis
Fennica 26:16-24
Hayman, Peter (1990): Birdwatcher’s Pocket
Guide. London, p. 69
Hentschel, Erwin & Wagner, Günther
1990): Zoologisches Wörterbuch. Jena, p. 441
Hess, Albert (1926): Von der Nahrungssuche
des Mauerseglers. Ornithologische Monatsschrift 51:174-175
Kaiser, Erich (1992): Populationsdynamik
einer Mauersegler- Apus apus Kolonie unter besonderer Berücksichtigung
der Nichtbrüter. Die Vogelwelt 113:71-81
Karte von Berlin 1:4000 No. 4131, ed.
(1987) Bezirksamt Neukölln, Vermessungsamt
Karte von Berlin 1:4000 No. 4135, ed.
(1984) Der Senator für Bau- und Wohnungswesen V
Koskimies, Jukka (1961): Delayed departure
of the swift (Apus apus) from Finland in the autumn of 1957. Ornis
Fennica 38:105-127
Lack, David (1956): Swifts in a Tower.
London
Lack, David (1958): The return and departure
of Swifts at Oxford. Ibis 100:477-502
Lack, David & Lack, Elizabeth (1952):
The Breeding Behaviour of the Swift. British Birds 45:193-195
Lack, David & Owen, D.F. (1955): The
food of the swift. Journal of Animal Ecology 24:120- 136
Lakowitz (1925): Zur Frage „Mauersegler
auf dem Erdboden“. Ornithologische Monatsschrift 50:45-46
Lawrence, Eleanor (Ed.) 1995): Dictionary
of Biological Terms. Harlow (Essex), p. 591
Offringa, Henk (1994): Gierzwaluweninventarisatie
in Wageningen 1990. Pennevluchten 12:4- 11
Parmenter & Owen (1954): The Swift,
Apus apus L., as a predator of flies. Journal of the Society for
British Entomology 5:27-33
Reuter, Barbro Nedstam & Ruotanen,
Tarmo-Oskari (1980): Observations of interlocked
Swifts Apus apus and A. affinis.
Ornis Fennica 57:132-133
Ries in Stadler (1917): Vom Zug der Mauersegler
(Micropus apus apus [L.]) im Maintal 1916. Verhandlungen der Ornithologischen
Gesellschaft in Bayern 13:74-86
Roper, Edward N. (1960): Swifts fighting.
British Birds 53:447
Sliwinski, Ursula (1938): Isopiptesen
einiger Vogelarten in Europa. Zoologica Poloniae 2:249- 287
Swaen, A.E.H. (1922): Bijzonderheden betreffende
de Gierzwaluw. Ardea 11:117-123
Tigges, Ulrich (1994): Beobachtungen am
Mauersegler (Apus apus) und Bericht über eine Spätbrut
1993. Berliner ornithologischer Bericht 4:129-141
Tigges, Ulrich (1995): Kann man Mauersegler
gezielt ansiedeln? Der Falke 42:250-252
Tombal, Christine & Tombal, Jean-Charles
(1995): Recensement des colonies de Martinet noir (Apus apus) dans
le sud du Département du Nord en juillet 1994. Héron 28:2-25
Trouche, Lucius (1948): Le martinet noir
Apus apus L. dans la région d’Arles. Alauda 16:
233-235
Weitnauer, Emil (1990): Mein Vogel. Oltingen
BL. (Basellandschaftlicher Natur- und Vogelschutzverband), p. 88
Weitnauer, Emil & Scherner, Erwin
R. (1994): Apus apus - Mauersegler. In Glutz von Blotzheim &
Bauer: Handbuch der Vögel Mitteleuropas Vol. 9, p. 671-712,
Wiesbaden |